放屁很臭是什么原因| 什么的豆角| 适当是什么意思| 什么是沙龙| 喝牛奶拉肚子是什么原因| 隆字五行属什么| 黄体可能是什么意思啊| 晰字五行属什么| 净值是什么| 前列腺炎需要做什么检查| 降压药什么时候吃| 天赦日是什么意思| 广东属于什么气候| 少年班是什么意思| 阴历九月是什么星座| 尽性是什么意思| ctc是什么| 软科是什么意思| 胆结石吃什么药可以化掉结石| 猫咪打呼噜代表什么| 火头鱼是什么鱼| 为什么腹部隐隐作痛| 太阳为什么能一直燃烧| 143是什么意思| 暑假是什么时候| uv是什么材质| 老人不睡觉是什么预兆| 右冠优势型是什么意思| 什么叫等离子| 为什么油耳朵就有狐臭| 百合花代表什么意思| 海盐是什么盐| 胸部ct平扫能检查出什么| swisse是什么药| 小腿怕冷是什么原因| 毫无意义是什么意思| 花园里面有什么| ca125检查是什么意思| 哺乳期可以吃什么水果| 一是什么意思| 狗狗流眼泪是什么原因| maxco是什么牌子| 胃气胃胀吃什么药最好| 什么动物有四个胃| 老人手抖是什么原因| 急性胃炎吃什么药| 虐猫是什么意思| 夏至吃什么传统食物| 淋巴细胞减少说明什么| 用什么洗头白发能变黑| izzue是什么牌子| 艾滋病什么症状| 胯骨疼是什么原因| 博五行属性是什么| 祖马龙是什么档次| 为什么来月经| 冰粉为什么要加石灰水| 煤气罐在什么情况下会爆炸| 碱水是什么| 履约是什么意思| 凉粉是用什么做的| 精神慰藉什么意思| 口香糖是什么材料做的| 睡眠质量不好挂什么科| 指甲不平有凹凸是什么原因| fabric是什么面料| 手麻吃什么药| b像什么| 为什么老是恶心想吐| 如泰山前面一个字是什么啊| 贝伐珠单抗是什么药| 凉粉是什么做的| 阴道口瘙痒是什么原因| alt是什么| 平平仄仄是什么意思| 门庭是什么意思| 党参和丹参有什么区别| whan是什么意思| 护发素什么牌子好| 什么车可以闯红灯| 电磁炉用什么锅最好| 甲鱼和什么一起炖最好| 可见原始心管搏动是什么意思| 癸未日五行属什么| siv是什么意思| 什么的表达| 什么是黄褐斑| 4点是什么时辰| 1014是什么星座| 蟑螂为什么会飞| 黑色素是什么| 蝉为什么会叫| 继发性不孕是什么意思| 感冒发烧吃点什么食物比较好| 甲状腺病变是什么意思| 秉字五行属什么| 尿检阳性是什么意思| 皮粉色是什么颜色| 热泪盈眶的盈是什么意思| 女生被口是什么感觉| 呼吸道感染吃什么药最好| 抽烟头晕是什么原因| 自缢死亡是什么意思| 什么的流水| 男人下面有异味什么原因| 牙齿发软是什么原因| 头左边痛是什么原因| 屁股疼什么原因| 阴婚是什么意思| 下眼皮肿是什么原因| 拉油便是什么原因| 胃有幽门螺旋杆菌是什么症状| 上坟可以带什么水果| 南岳什么山| 肾结石发作有什么症状| 低血压的人吃什么好| 潭柘寺求什么最灵验| 窦性心律不齐吃什么药| dna里面有什么| 妍五行属性是什么| tia是什么病| 右手发抖是什么原因| 宝宝吐奶是什么原因引起的| 1994年属什么| 什么的什么是什么的伞| 近视是什么原因造成的| 割痔疮后吃什么恢复快| 红色的月亮是什么征兆| 无语什么意思| 冲锋衣三合一是什么意思| 海参不适合什么人吃| 孟子姓什么| 扶乩是什么意思| 趋光性是什么意思| 心脏t波改变是什么意思| 涮菜都有什么菜| 看头发挂什么科| 硬化萎缩性苔藓是什么病| 湿疹不能吃什么| 迅雷不及掩耳之势是什么意思| 约稿是什么意思| 女人出轨有什么表现| 女人吃桃子有什么好处和坏处| 穆斯林不吃什么| 西瓜可以做什么饮料| 蟹爪兰用什么肥料最好| 肺癌有什么症状| 1985属什么| 纪检是干什么的| 2002年出生属什么| 为什么会血压高| 女人肾虚是什么原因| 西游记什么时候拍的| 脚底褪皮是什么原因| hbsag阴性是什么意思| 川崎病是什么原因引起的| 洁身自好什么意思| 物以类聚形容什么动物| 刺猬是什么动物| 内膜厚是什么原因引起的| 前列腺是什么器官| 肉字五行属什么| ct什么意思| 米白色是什么颜色| 脂肪肝喝什么茶最好| 什么是处男| 苡米和薏米有什么区别| 静脉血栓有什么症状| 819是什么意思| 后背沉重感是什么原因引起的| 怀孕吃什么| 身体上有小红点是什么病| 意下如何什么意思| 喝什么养胃| 高压氧治疗有什么作用| 生殖细胞是什么| 干事是什么级别| 脾主四肢是什么意思| 什么是早泄| 吃什么药马上硬起来| 是谁在敲打我窗是什么歌| 病态是什么意思| 大队长是什么级别| 狗狗感冒了是什么症状| 忽然流鼻血是什么原因引起的| 吃什么有助于骨头恢复| 肝内钙化灶是什么意思| 惊厥是什么症状| 唐氏综合征是什么原因造成的| pad是什么| 疑神疑鬼是什么意思| x58主板配什么cpu| 肝的反射区在什么部位| 黑色的裤子配什么颜色的上衣| 在是什么意思| 甲亢是什么症状| 熟地黄是什么| 鼻鼽病是什么意思| 儿童舌系带短挂什么科| 手指尖麻木是什么原因| 为什么会得炎症| 什么蔬菜补铁| 正财透干是什么意思| 望尘莫及是什么意思| 什么将什么相| 党按照什么的原则选拔干部| 出什么入什么| kaiser是什么品牌| 胃疼需要做什么检查| 乱伦是什么| 肌红蛋白是什么意思| 眩晕症是什么原因| 血脂高吃什么能降下来| 月经来有血块是什么原因| 下压高是什么原因引起的| 病人说胡话是什么征兆| 男孩什么时候开始发育| 什么是双高| 孩子积食吃什么药| 呋喃硫胺片治什么病| 白茶是什么茶| 类风湿关节炎吃什么好| 肠胃不好吃什么药| 什么人不能吃西洋参| 经常抽筋是什么原因| 迷妹是什么意思| 晚上1点是什么时辰| 睡觉多梦是什么原因| 须发早白吃什么中成药| 什么样的白带是怀孕了| cheblo空调是什么牌子| 女性尿检能查出什么病| 医保报销需要什么材料| 10月19日什么星座| bally什么牌子| 芙字五行属什么| 避孕药是什么原理| 肺炎衣原体和支原体有什么区别| 天蝎座和什么座最配| 吃避孕药对身体有什么影响| 肝不好吃什么药| 广东话扑街是什么意思| 双肺门不大是什么意思| 苹果对身体有什么好处| 湿气重去医院挂什么科| 蚱蜢吃什么食物| 上嘴唇发白是因为什么原因| 白醋泡脚有什么好处| lemon是什么意思| 感冒了吃什么水果比较好| 养阴生津是什么意思| 靴型心见于什么病| 入户口需要什么资料| 什么是前鼻音和后鼻音| 什么地大喊| 狗狗蛋蛋红肿是什么原因| 什么是生理盐水| 银梳子梳头有什么好处和坏处| 素面朝天什么生肖| 小儿支气管炎咳嗽吃什么药好得快| moda是什么牌子| 杜鹃花什么颜色| ev病毒是什么| 狗吃什么蔬菜好| 百度
Tansley review
Free Access

技术创新为内容建设添翼

Agnes S. Dellinger
百度 周鹏突破抛投命中,赵睿突破上篮也中,西热力江造三分犯规三罚三中,赵睿补篮得手,首节结束,新疆16比35落后。

Corresponding Author

Agnes S. Dellinger

University of Vienna, Rennweg 14, Vienna, 1030 Austria

Author for correspondence:

Agnes S. Dellinger

Tel: +43 660 3572098

Email: [email protected]

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 07 July 2020
Citations: 151

Summary

Pollination syndromes, recurring suites of floral traits appearing in connection with specific functional pollinator groups, have served for decades to organise floral diversity under a functional–ecological perspective. Some potential caveats, such as over-simplification of complex plant–animal interactions or lack of empirical observations, have been identified and discussed in recent years. Which of these caveats do indeed cause problems, which have been solved and where do future possibilities lie? I address these questions in a review of the pollination-syndrome literature of 2010 to 2019. I show that the majority of studies was based on detailed empirical pollinator observations and could reliably predict pollinators based on a few floral traits such as colour, shape or reward. Some traits (i.e. colour) were less reliable in predicting pollinators than others (i.e. reward, corolla width), however. I stress that future studies should consider floral traits beyond those traditionally recorded to expand our understanding of mechanisms of floral evolution. I discuss statistical methods suitable for objectively analysing the interplay of system-specific evolutionary constraints, pollinator-mediated selection and adaptive trade-offs at microecological and macroecological scales. I exemplify my arguments on an empirical dataset of floral traits of a neotropical plant radiation in the family Melastomataceae.

Contents
Summary 1
I. Introduction 1
II. A critical review of the pollination-syndrome literature from 2010 to 2019 2
III. Merianieae as a case study – pollination syndromes and flower evolution 16
IV. Concluding remarks 17
Acknowledgements

17

References 17

I. Introduction

Flowers are the defining structures of angiosperms (Sauquet & Magallón, 2018). While their organisation (bauplan) is relatively conserved across larger taxonomic units, flowers represent an astounding diversity in mode, i.e. architecture, colour, reward and scent, even among closely related species (Endress, 1996; Specht & Bartlett, 2009; Schiestl & Johnson, 2013; Sauquet et al., 2017). This diversity in floral mode has largely been attributed to the selection imposed by pollinating agents, and, to a lesser extent, by floral antagonists and abiotic factors (Strauss & Whitall, 2006; Harder & Johnson, 2009; van der Niet et al., 2014; Gervasi & Schiestl, 2017; Campbell et al., 2018; Caruso et al., 2018). Over the past 150 years, the concept of pollination syndromes has been formulated and continuously refined to serve as a large-scale framework for structuring this diversity in floral mode from a functional, pollination-ecological perspective (Delpino, 1873–1874; Vogel, 1954; Stebbins, 1970; Faegri & van der Pijl, 1979; Endress, 1996; Ollerton et al., 2009; Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014). Generally, pollination syndromes represent convergent floral adaptations to specific functional pollinator groups (Smith & Kriebel, 2018; Dellinger et al., 2019c). Importantly, there is no strict definition on how to delimit syndromes (e.g. which traits, which pollinator groups) and different authors have employed different classification systems throughout the years (reviewed in Fenster et al., 2004; Ollerton et al., 2009).

Regardless of the delimitation, the fundamental assumption of the pollination-syndrome concept is that flowers adapt to their single most efficient functional pollinator group (most efficient in removing and depositing pollen, see definition of ‘efficiency’ by Ne'eman et al., 2009; Stebbins, 1970, Ashwort et al., 2015). A functional pollinator group, as such, is defined as pollinators who select for the same combination of floral traits, while different functional groups will select for different trait combinations (Fenster et al., 2004). Recent meta-analyses on pollination syndromes (Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014; Ashworth et al., 2015) have used the following 11 functional groups: bee, bird, bat, fly, wasp, moth, butterfly, long-tongued fly, beetle, carrion fly and nonflying mammal. From here onwards, I refer to these as ‘traditional syndromes’ (also see discussion by Ollerton et al., 2009).

Pollination syndromes are based on floral traits that are likely to underlie pollinator-mediated selection and capture differences in activity patterns, sensory abilities, dietary preferences, morphology and behaviour of pollinators (Fenster et al., 2004; Muchhala, 2007; Schiestl & Johnson, 2013). Floral traits that are usually recorded (in traditional syndromes) include the timing of anthesis, flower colour and scent, flower orientation, flower size and symmetry, overall corolla shape, the position of sexual organs and the reward type (for a summary see Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014). Some studies also include flower exposure from foliage, sturdiness, subtler quantitative colour differences (e.g. UV patterning) or the more refined analyses of scent bouquets and nectar composition (Raguso et al., 2003; Reynolds et al., 2009).

As a means of classifying flowers under a functional–ecological perspective, pollination syndromes have often been used to predict pollinators for species for which pollinators are not known. These predictions were generally made by the experienced researcher by sorting flowers into syndromes based on the observed trait combinations (Faegri & van der Pijl, 1979). Over the past decades, more objective statistical classification methods have become available (Wilson et al., 2004; Pichler et al., 2020). The strongest associations between functional pollinator groups and flower traits have been reported for highly specialised systems in the tropics (Ashworth et al., 2015).

The concept of pollination syndromes has received considerable attention and stimulated controversial debate in recent years (e.g. Waser et al., 1996, 2011; Martén-Rodríguez et al., 2009; Ollerton et al., 2009; Armbruster et al., 2011; Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014; Prieto-Benítez et al., 2015). Much of the controversy related to pollination syndromes arose from the urgent desire of botanists and pollination biologists to devise a broadly applicable functional classification and explanation for the overwhelming diversity of flowers, thereby disregarding subtler aspects of pollination biology. Some shortfalls have been identified and discussed extensively (Ollerton et al., 2009; Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014), including the over-simplification of complex plant–animal interactions (i.e. lumping all visitors as pollinators regardless of differences in their efficiency in transferring pollen), the predominant focus on specialised systems only, or using syndromes to predict pollinators in plant clades in which no empirical pollinator observations are available. Further, the lack of a unified terminology, incongruences in geographic (e.g. worldwide vs regional) and taxonomic scale (e.g. across angiosperms vs single species) as well as the limited focus on a few, easy-to-code-for, floral traits make comparability between studies difficult and may render syndrome predictions unreliable (Ollerton et al., 2009; Dellinger et al., 2019a). Recent studies have shown a potential remedy against some of these shortcomings. The detailed assessment of pollination efficiency of a flower’s visitor assemblage, for example, was found to be a reliable measure to correctly identify the ‘most efficient pollinator’ and hence correctly predict pollination syndromes (Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014). Also, more detailed and inclusive recording of floral traits helped to differentiate syndromes with higher accuracy and gave more information on syndrome evolution (Abrahamczyk et al., 2017).

Pollination syndromes continue to stir controversial debate, however, with researchers questioning their validity and heuristic value in the (post)genomic era. Keeping all caveats in mind, how reliable and useful are pollination syndromes in predicting pollinators and understanding floral evolution? How have researchers dealt with the known caveats in recent years and which caveats/problems prevail? And how may modern microecological and macroecological and evolutionary questions make use of the syndrome concept?

In the following sections, I present a critical review of the current pollination-syndrome literature (2010–2019) in the light of the known shortcomings. First, I show that pollination syndromes are applied across a broad spectrum of disciplines, rendering a robust conceptual framework an absolute necessity. Then, I quantify how often pollination syndromes are used without knowledge of pollination biology, how pollinators are recorded and which traits are used most often to substantiate syndrome predictions. I further analyse how often predictions are erroneous and which traits are considered as most or least informative in differentiating syndromes. I discuss and summarise a variety of approaches that have been employed to objectively classify pollination syndromes and study flower diversification. Finally, I demonstrate what we may learn from the combination of a broader assessment of floral traits, multivariate statistics and comparative phylogenetic methods on an empirical trait dataset of the Melastomataceae tribe Merianieae. I conclude this review with an outlook on the opportunities to adopt new approaches for studying pollination syndromes.

II. A critical review of the pollination-syndrome literature from 2010 to 2019

The first millennial decade saw a number of highly influential conceptual publications on common shortfalls and possible improvements of the pollination syndrome concept, i.e. Fenster et al.'s (2004) review on functional pollinator groups and Ollerton et al.'s (2009) critique on the (mis)use of pollination syndromes. What solutions have we sought since then to overcome these shortfalls, and where do current problems lie? To answer these questions, I conducted a literature search with the search terms ‘pollination&syndrome*’, considering title, abstract, author keywords and keywords plus, in the year range 2010–2019 in Web of Science 5.3.4. I screened 447 papers to extract information on the scope of the study, the approaches used to study pollinators, traits recorded and the types of analyses employed (Supporting Information Methods S1, S2). I excluded 101 papers that did not directly address pollination syndromes (e.g. theoretical or methodological articles, taxonomic descriptions, reviews), from further analyses, leaving 346 articles.

I first give an overview of the variety of objectives addressed by studies employing pollination syndromes. Then, working through eight major points of criticism on the pollination-syndrome concept, I quantify (when possible) and discuss whether and how these shortfalls may impact and bias our understanding of plant–pollinator interactions and floral evolution. Each section briefly introduces the point of criticism, provides an evaluation of the associated problems and is followed by a discussion on possible solutions.

1. The objectives of studies employing pollination syndromes

In the 346 papers reviewed, pollination syndromes were used in a broad variety of contexts and questions. I scored the major objectives of each study (each study may have multiple objectives; hence percentages of categories do not sum to 100%). The majority of studies aimed at generally describing the study system’s pollination syndrome (52.6%), testing the concept (33.8%) and experimentally assessing, for example, the breeding system or relative effectiveness of different pollinators (30.3%). Here 21.7% of studies investigated whether pollination syndromes correlated with other factors such as the availability of flowering resources and 17.3% used syndromes to analyse whether and how pollinators affect/correlate with clade diversification. Pollination syndromes were also used to address conservation issues (7.8%), modelling and statistical prediction of pollinators (3.8% and 2.3%, respectively); 5.8% sought to better understand the evolution of pollination syndromes at the genomic level (e.g. evolution of colour genes).

2. The problem of scale

Traditional pollination syndromes were developed as a broad, functional–ecological classification system that would be applicable across angiosperms. At this broadest possible taxonomic scale, it follows that more general categorisations (e.g. widely distributed floral traits, which can be scored for all taxa) are used. It is not surprising that researchers have found mismatches between these idealised, traditional syndromes and the trait combinations realised in actual flowers (Ollerton et al., 2009). Furthermore, it has become clear that pollination syndromes differ in different parts of the world (e.g. the ‘bat’ and ‘bird’ syndrome in the Old and New world tropics are characterised by different trait combinations, Fleming et al., 2009). Unless the aim of a study is to test whether a (group of) plant species fits into ‘traditional’ syndromes, the overall conclusion was that studies at lower taxonomic scales (e.g. within families) should focus on taxon-specific traits to accurately describe syndromes in the group and region under study (Ollerton et al., 2009).

The 346 papers screened indeed addressed pollination syndromes at various taxonomic scales (Fig. 1a). The majority, 59.8%, worked at small taxonomic units (single or few species), 21.4% addressed questions at generic, tribal or family level, 15.9% worked at the community level and the remaining 2.9% worked at other/higher scales. In total, 244 studies (70.5%) included floral traits. The majority of these (77.5%) recorded one or several of the traits included in traditional syndromes (as listed by Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014; Table S2; Fig. 1b); 90.6% included one or several additional traits such as quantitative measurements of nectar, scent, colour or flower morphology, pollen grain features, pollen to ovule ratios or shape of nectaries. Of the c. 10% of studies that did not include additional traits, the majority (37.8%) were community-level studies including many different taxa, followed by studies on single taxa or studies at the generic/tribal/family level (29.7% each).

Details are in the caption following the image
Pollination-syndrome studies worked at different scales (a), were based on relatively few traits (b) and used different approaches to record pollinators (c–f). (a) The majority of studies worked at low taxonomic scales or the community context. (b) The majority of studies recorded relatively few traditional syndrome traits or quantitative measurements of reward, scent and corolla. (c) Most studies empirically observed pollinators or used literature reports to classify functional groups, c. 7% was only based on syndrome inference (floral traits). (d) More than 90% of studies quantitatively recorded pollinators (VR, visitation rate; PQ – pollinator quality (i.e. pollen deposition); other PQ, other approach to differentiate visitors from pollinators (i.e. morphological match between flower and pollinator; Supporting Information Methods S1); type, only recording functional group). (e) More than 60% of studies reported visitation by more than one functional pollinator group; ‘one or more’ refers to studies including more than one plant species. (f) More than 60% of studies reported only a subset of the observed visitors as effective pollinators.

These results suggested that particularly researchers working at small taxonomic scales have heeded the call to use more refined trait datasets to evaluate pollination syndromes. Ultimately, the lack of a single ‘scale’ at which pollination syndromes operate may turn out to be a strength rather than a weakness. As long as authors are clear about the scale at which they work, it offers the necessary flexibility to adjust character coding to functionally meaningful traits of the specific study system. In the long run, this may even prove valuable in understanding universal patterns of pollinator-mediated selection and angiosperm evolution (and hence angiosperm-wide pollination syndromes) as new traits and pollinator combinations hitherto not considered may emerge as important (also see section II.7 ‘There is more to pollination syndromes than a handful of floral traits’).

3. The predominant focus on (few) specialised systems

Obviously, pollination syndromes were conceived for lineages comprising taxa with markedly diverging flower traits specialised on different functional pollinator groups. This focus on specialised systems came in for criticism as a large number of angiosperms indeed is generalised on being pollinated by several functional groups (Waser et al., 1996).

In the literature reviewed, species from 76 different plant families were considered (Fig. 2; Table S1; note that I excluded community-level studies and studies at higher taxonomic or nonsystematic scales from this count). This represents 18.9% of the 416 angiosperm families currently recognised (The Angiosperm Phylogeny Group et al., 2016). The plant families studied were mostly groups well known for enigmatic, specialised pollination syndromes, such as Orchidaceae (24 studies, Monocots), Gesneriaceae (17 studies), Solanaceae (i.e. Petunia, 17 studies), Plantaginaceae (i.e. Penstemon, 14 studies; all Lamiids) and Fabaceae (13 studies, Fabids). Some of the largest families, like Asteraceae, were only represented by a few (two) studies, and families known for generalised pollination systems such as Apiaceae or Brassicaceae were not included at all.

Details are in the caption following the image
Here, c. 20% of angiosperm families were represented in syndrome studies, with a strong focus on specialised interactions in the Lamiids and Monocots. Flower images represent enigmatic systems (clockwise, starting with Magnolids: Annonaceae, Orchidaceae, Ranunculaceae, Melastomataceae, Fabaceae, Plantaginaceae, Solanaceae, Gesneriaceae, Campanulaceae); numbers in brackets give the number of studies conducted in each clade. Coloration of major clades follows Cole et al. (2017) (Supporting Information Methods S3).

Quantifying our potential perception bias arising from the focus on (few) lineages with ‘exciting’ pollination biology will only be possible in future once we know more about hitherto neglected lineages and systems with seemingly ‘boring’ (often generalised) flowers (Vasconcelos et al., 2019). Albeit not included in the literature reviewed, recent studies on generalised systems do exist (e.g. Gómez et al., 2014, 2016; Zych et al., 2019). These have documented geographic patterning in floral traits, and could partially link it with adaptation to the local pollinator community. Studies performed under experimental conditions could further demonstrate strong selection on scent mediated by a subset of the pollinator community even in seemingly generalised flowers (Gervasi & Schiestl, 2017). Investigating how, why and when generalised systems, and perhaps a ‘generalist syndrome’, are maintained despite specialisation on the local pollinator community will be one important approach to acquiring a more holistic understanding of the mechanisms underlying syndrome formation (Aigner, 2006). Furthermore, studying large plant groups with phenotypically ‘boring’ flowers is essential to understand potential intrinsic (system specific) and extrinsic (e.g. abiotic and biotic environment) constraints which may have prevented floral diversification despite species diversification. Finally, the investigation of ‘new’ plant groups whose pollination biology is understudied may lead to exciting discoveries of novel pollination systems and associated floral trait changes such as pollination by marine invertebrates (van Tussenbroek et al., 2016) or pollinator shifts in functionally highly specialised systems (Dellinger et al., 2019a).

4. Pollinator observations and classifying pollinators into ‘functional groups’

Different pollinator species are pooled into ‘functional groups’ based on how they interact with flowers (Fenster et al., 2004). Such groups may include a single species or many different species (Armbruster, 2017). Again, while it may be useful to work with the traditional categories (sensu Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014) at high taxonomic levels, the majority of studies investigates syndromes at lower taxonomic levels (Fig. 1a) and may find a more refined categorisation useful (Murúa & Espíndola, 2015). Only empirical observations allow us to correctly understand flower–pollinator interactions and hence devise meaningful functional categories (if necessary), disregarding taxonomic affinities (Fenster et al., 2009a). How many current studies are indeed based on empirical pollinator observations and how have they classified pollinators?

In total, 315 of the 346 studies used functional groups for analyses. Of these, 54.9% had performed pollinator observations in the specific study and 32.4% were based on pollinator observations from the literature (Fig. 1c). Only 7% relied on inferences made solely based on floral traits (syndromes) and the remaining 5.7% used combinations of observations, literature reports and syndrome inference. Here, 83.3% of the studies based on direct pollinator observations (a total of 173) worked at low taxonomic levels (one or a few species) and primarily aimed at describing and testing/verifying pollination syndromes (73.3%). Most of these studies summarised pollinators into traditional functional groups despite identifying pollinators at the lowest taxonomic level possible (often species). Only 4.4% of studies created unusual functional groups based on observed or presumed behavioural or morphological differences between pollinators, such as phytophagous vs dung beetles (Schiestl & D?tterl, 2012), 15 insect groups (Koski et al., 2015) or straight-bill and sickle-bill hummingbirds (Lagomarsino et al., 2017).

These results showed that a large proportion of (particularly small-scale) studies indeed verified syndromes through empirical pollinator observations and only a small fraction of studies worked without empirical data. This does not mean, however, that we may reduce efforts to document pollinators. Several of the large-scale studies included in this review suffered from deficient pollinator observations and hence could not systematically analyse flower character evolution in relation to pollination (e.g. Reginato & Michelangeli, 2016; Heckenhauer et al., 2018; Vasconcelos et al., 2019). This observation is particularly important in the light of modern sequencing techniques and online search engines, which allow for the fast collation of large datasets, but leave us uninformed of the functional–ecological implications of flower mode (Ollerton et al., 2015).

Regarding classification of functional groups, most studies adhered to traditional or taxonomic groups to summarise observed patterns. It is unclear whether a different grouping would have led to different conclusions. However, the fact that many authors have identified pollinators to species is promising, as it allows more refined assessments of flower–pollinator relationships (Murúa & Espíndola, 2015; Pichler et al., 2020). In the past, only a few authors have made use of the flexibility of the functional-group idea in that it allows (and actually demands!) grouping based on the interaction differences observed in the study system, irrespective of taxonomic affinities of the pollinators. For example, Wilson et al. (2004) grouped individuals of the same bee species into nectar foragers vs pollen foragers and Armbruster (1993) differentiated fragrance-collecting male bees from resin-collecting female euglossine bees. At the other perspective, pollinators that we intuitively perceive as different may sometimes function as one group (e.g. nectar foraging hummingbirds, bats, rodents, flower piercers; Dellinger et al., 2019a).

Generally, we have to be aware of the fact that any typological categorisation, such as functional groups and syndromes, leaves us at the boundaries of subjective interpretation of continuous, dynamic natural systems. While functional groups have been shown to be highly useful to summarise patterns, I urge that we continue to move on from typology to the (objective) quantification of syndromes when possible. I propose two approaches towards delineating functional pollinator groups and making classification less typological. The first approach starts with the pollinators. Quantifying the contribution of (different) flower visitors to pollination is the most powerful tool for understanding their importance as pollinators (see section II.5 ‘Visitation by multiple functional groups and selection by the most efficient pollinator’). Pollination efficiency may be combined with measurements of pollinator morphology (e.g. length of mouthparts). These matrices may be combined with floral trait matrices to search for convergence patterns (Guzmán et al., 2017, Pichler et al., 2020) and may then be evaluated in the light of known or observed sensory/behavioural differences of pollinators to devise functional groups. Manipulative experiments may be conducted subsequently to identify the factors which render a specific set of taxa a ‘functional’ group and when and how this group may be narrowed or widened (e.g. through trait manipulation restricting pollen transfer). The second approach starts from the flowers. Particularly when working at macroevolutionary scales, authors may not want to classify flowers a priori but rather use statistical methods (Table 2) to detect patterns of floral trait convergence, and only then relate these to pollinator data (Pichler et al., 2020).

5. Visitation by multiple functional groups and selection by the ‘most efficient pollinator’

When formulating the ‘most effective pollinator principle’, Stebbins (1970) emphasised that ‘character syndromes’ (pollination syndromes) do not preclude the existence of secondary, potentially less efficient pollinators. Indeed, most flowers are visited by multiple functional pollinator groups (e.g. Waser et al., 1996; Ollerton et al., 2007). One major advancement that pollination-syndrome research has seen was to acknowledge this fact and to start measuring the relative contribution to pollination (pollination efficiency) of (diverse) pollinator assemblages instead of only documenting their flower visitation (Fenster et al., 2004; Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014; Santiago-Hernández et al., 2019). In specialised, and even in many generalised, flowers (which are easily accessible to various different pollinator groups), only a subset of all visitors was found to be effective as pollinators (Barto? et al., 2005; Rosas-Guerrero et al., 2014). Recording pollinator efficiency requires more time and potentially laboratory equipment than assessing visitation only. How many authors have employed more refined measures to identifying ‘most efficient pollinators’ and what have they found? Does visitation by multiple visitors make syndrome predictions less accurate?

Of the 173 studies based on direct observations, the majority (63.2%) included both an evaluation of visitation rate and pollen transfer efficiency (Fig. 1d). In total, 18.1% only documented visitation rates, 11.1% only documented pollen transfer and 7.6% only reported types of pollinators. As expected, the majority of studies (61.2%) found more than two functional groups visiting; 13.4% reported visitation by two groups and 25.4% documented only one functional group visiting (Fig. 1e). The vast majority of studies documenting two or more functional groups as visitors reported only a subset of these visitors as pollinators (68.8% and 93.8%, respectively); 71.4% of studies reported the most frequent visitor also being the most important pollinator (but also see King et al., 2013); 122 studies formulated a hypothesis about an expected pollinator/visitor, based either on previous observations or expectations derived from floral syndrome traits (see below). Here, 85.2% could at least partially confirm these expectations, while 13.9% found other pollinators/visitors than expected and one study did not find any legitimate pollinator. Syndrome expectations were confirmed or contradicted equally often, regardless of whether multiple functional groups or only one was visiting (83.5% at least partly confirmed syndrome expectation with multiple functional groups visiting and 90% when one functional group was visiting).

These results underline findings reported previously: visitation by multiple functional pollinator groups is common but does not negatively impact the predictive power of pollination syndromes. It will be interesting, however, to further evaluate the impact of secondary/less efficient pollinators on floral trait evolution. Generally, pollination by multiple functional pollinator groups is believed to be disadvantageous, as it may induce strong fitness costs through adaptive trade-offs (Muchhala, 2008; Pady?áková et al., 2013; Strelin et al., 2017). It seems, however, that not all pollinator combinations are equally costly and trade-offs may sometimes remain small (Aigner, 2006; Ashworth et al., 2015). An exciting and unanswered question in this context is whether some pollinator combinations (e.g. hummingbirds and bats, see Lagomarsino & Muchhala, 2019; Dellinger et al., 2019b) are more likely to evolve into stable bimodal systems (i.e. systems with intermediate adaptations to two distinct functional pollinator groups sensu Goldblatt and Manning, 2006) than others (e.g. hummingbirds and bees, Castellanos et al., 2004)? Why and how is bimodality maintained and under which conditions does generalisation evolve?

Furthermore, despite the predominance of pollinators as selective agents on flowers (Caruso et al., 2018), other biotic interactions (e.g. herbivores, competition) are also important (Jogesh et al., 2017). In the reviewed literature, only eight studies addressed and quantified other agents of selection than pollinators: six studied floral herbivory and two addressed reward robbing. Particularly when pollinators and antagonists are attracted through the same traits (e.g. large, colourful displays, Brothers & Atwell, 2014), this may induce conflicting selection on these traits and potentially lead to the evolution of defence/deterrence traits (Doubleday et al., 2013). Incorporating antagonistic interactions (i.e. through fitness estimates) and potential defence traits into studies on pollination syndromes is important since defence traits may promote (preadaptations) or constrain shifts between pollination syndromes (Hanley et al., 2009) and may also explain mismatches between traditional and observed syndromes (see section II.8 ‘Not all floral traits are equally informative’).

6. Prediction of pollinators

Particularly in large plant clades in which time, logistic and budgetary constraints limit the number of species for which pollinators can be observed empirically, pollination syndromes are used for predicting pollinators (Lagomarsino et al., 2017; Dellinger et al., 2019a). Several authors have argued, however, that such predictions are risky, particularly when no (or few) pollinator observations exist for the respective plant group, when multiple functional groups visit flowers or when only very few floral traits are considered (Ollerton et al., 2009; Fenster et al., 2009a). In small-scale studies, pollination syndromes are often used to formulate a hypothesis about a species’ most likely pollinator that is then evaluated through measurements of pollinator efficiency. I extracted this information from the literature to assess how often authors verified/falsified their hypothesis and to understand which floral traits led them to their specific hypothesis.

In total, 64 studies hypothesised which functional group would pollinate the study species based on syndrome traits. Of those, 27.8% expressed doubt on classification into clear-cut syndromes because they observed floral trait combinations commonly assigned to different syndromes (Fig. 3a). Of those, 85% correctly predicted visitation by mixed assemblages, while 15% found different pollinators than expected from syndrome traits (Fig. 3b,c). Studies that did not express uncertainty about syndrome classification could (at least partially) confirm predictions in 86.3% of cases and 13.6% of studies reported other pollinators than expected (Fig. 3b,c). In this regard, 58 studies substantiated their syndrome expectations by naming one to three (traditional) syndrome traits in the introduction or materials and methods sections: colour (55.2%), reward (34.5%), scent (31.0%) and flower shape (25.9%) being the most commonly used traits (Fig. 4; Table S2).

Details are in the caption following the image
Pollination syndromes could be used to correctly predict pollinators in c. 85% of studies. (a) Studies expressing doubt on clear-cut syndromes due to unusual trait combinations or no doubt due to matching of traditional syndrome traits. (b) Studies confirmed or contradicted syndromes equally often. (c) Pollinator generalisation or specialization did not impact the predictive potential of syndromes.
Details are in the caption following the image
Floral traits named to substantiate syndrome prediction. Colour, reward, scent and flower shape were used most often to justify syndrome expectations; erroneous predictions are indicated by black bars.

These results demonstrated that authors could predict pollinators with high accuracy even when basing predictions on only a few traits. Importantly, these traits also allowed authors to correctly identify species that were likely to be pollinated by multiple functional groups. How can these results be extrapolated to larger scales for which empirical verification of syndrome assumptions is not possible? Promising advancements have been made, particularly in the field of Machine Learning (Pichler et al., 2020). A series of powerful and mathematically flexible algorithms exists that may first be trained and evaluated on species for which empirical pollinator observations exist, and then be used to predict pollinators for (closely) related species that lack pollinator observations (Table 1; Dellinger et al., 2019a; Pichler et al., 2020).

Table 1. Methods for data acquisition – compilation of floral traits which may be studied under a pollination biological perspective to expand our understanding of pollination syndromes.
Trait Parameters to measure Possible methods for data acquisition Type of floral material needed Examples for functional significance in pollination
Anthesis Start time, duration Documentation in the field Fresh flowers Filter on activity pattern of pollinator, traditional syndrome character
Flower exposure Relative position to foliage Documentation in the field, from photos or herbarium specimens Fresh flowers, photos or herbarium vouchers Advertisement (Fenster et al., 2015; Diniz et al., 2019), accessibility filter, traditional syndrome character
Flower orientation Quantitative or qualitative (e.g. horizontal, vertical; Fenster et al., 2009) Documentation in the field, from photos or herbarium specimens Fresh flowers, photos or herbarium specimens Advertisement, efficiency (Fenster et al., 2009b; Wang et al., 2015), response to abiotic conditions, traditional syndrome character
Flower colour Reflectance/absorption spectra, UV-patterns, colour contrasts within different parts of the flower (e.g. stamens, pollen), qualitative classification UV-sensitive cameras, spectrophotometers (Johnson, 2013; van der Kooi et al., 2019), qualitative classification Fresh flowers, photos Advertisement, deterrence (reviewed in van der Kooi et al., 2019), traditional syndrome character
Surface structures of floral organs Measurements or classification of cell shapes Scanning electron microscopy or light microscopy (Papiorek et al., 2014; Dellinger et al., 2019a) Ethanol-preserved flower material Advertisement, deterrence (Comba et al., 2000), modulating the grip for small, alighting pollinators (Whitney et al., 2009)
Flower scent Characterisation and quantification of scent bouquets, qualitative classification Dynamic headspace techniques, gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (Burkle & Runyon, 2017), qualitative classification Fresh flowers Advertisement, deterrence (Schiestl & D?tterl, 2012; Burkle & Runyon, 2017)
Reward Reward type Qualitative classification Fresh flowers or ethanol-preserved material Advertisement, traditional syndrome character
Amount/volume e.g. nectar quantification by microcapillaries (Vandelook et al., 2019) Fresh flowers Advertisement (Katzer et al., 2019; Vandelook et al., 2019)
Availability (e.g. timing of nectar secretion) Documentation in the field Fresh flowers Advertisement (Maruyama et al., 2013)
Nutritive value, reward composition e.g. calorimetric analyses (Dellinger et al., 2014), High performance liquid chromatography for nectar sugar composition (Kr?mer et al., 2008) Collection of reward (e.g. preservation in ethanol) Advertisement (Vandelook et al., 2019; Dellinger et al., 2019b)
Exact location in flower Documentation in the field, flower sections and light microscopy (Dellinger et al., 2019b) Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (Armbruster et al., 2009)
Perianth (corolla) Size (diameter, petal length) Measurements, e.g. with callipers Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens Advertisement, traditional syndrome character
Fusion (e.g. tubes), constrictions (width : length) Measurements, e.g. with callipers Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens Advertisement, deterrence (Zung et al., 2015), efficiency (Muchhala, 2006)
Shape Qualitative classification; quantification through length measurements or geometric morphometrics (van der Niet et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2015; Strelin et al., 2017) Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, (herbarium specimens) Advertisement, deterrence, efficiency (Muchhala, 2007; Gómez et al., 2016; Smith & Kriebel, 2018; Benítez-Vieyra et al., 2019), traditional syndrome character
Androecium Heteranthery Qualitative classification FRESH or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens advertisement, efficiency (Velloso et al., 2018)
Position in flower Qualitative classification or quantification through (geometric) morphometric measurements Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens Advertisement (Sapir et al., 2017), efficiency (Armbruster et al., 2009; Xiao et al., 2017; Dellinger et al., 2019c)
Size (length and width of anthers, filaments etc.) Length measurements Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens Advertisement (Sapir et al., 2017), efficiency (Kudo, 2003)
Structure/texture of anthers Qualitative classification Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (Falc?o et al., 2016; Dellinger et al., 2019a)
Presence of appendages/trichomes/glands Qualitative classification or measurements Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens Advertisement, efficiency (Dellinger et al., 2014)
Timing of anther dehiscence, pollen dispensing schedules Qualitative classification, documentation in the field Fresh flowers Advertisement, efficiency (Castellanos et al., 2004; Xiao et al., 2017)
Mechanism of pollen transfer Qualitative classification Pollinator observations, artificial pollination experiments Efficiency (Wester & Classen-Bockhoff, 2007; Dellinger et al., 2014, 2019a)
Pollen Function ('pollen-dilemma'), is pollen also the reward? Qualitative classification Pollinator observations Advertisement (Dellinger et al., 2019a)
Amount Manual counting or use of a particle counter (e.g. Castellanos et al., 2004) Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (Cruden, 2000, but also see Lozada-Gobilard et al., 2018)
Quality (viability, protein content) Germination experiments (e.g. Carr et al., 2015) Anthers preserved in lactophenol with aniline blue Advertisement (Carr et al., 2015)
Pollen grain size and ornamentation Scanning electron microscopy or light microscopy (e.g. Konzmann et al., 2019) Ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (collectability of pollen e.g. Lunau et al., 2015; Konzmann et al., 2019)
Pollen kit, viscin threads e.g. Sudan dyes (Pacini & Hesse, 2005) Ethanol-preserved flower material Multifunctional (UV or water loss protection, pollen packaging; reviewed in Pacini & Hesse, 2005)
Gynoecium Timing of stigma receptivity Documentation in the field Fresh flowers Efficiency
Position of stigma(s) relative to stamens (site of pollen release) or reward Qualitative classification or length measurements or quantification through geometric morphometrics Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (Armbruster et al., 2009)
Stigma size or shape Qualitative classification or measurements, scanning electron microscopy Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material, herbarium specimens Efficiency (Cruden, 2000; Dellinger et al., 2019a)
Number of ovules Quantification through microscope or HRX-CT (Staedler et al., 2018) Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (Cruden, 2000, but also see Lozada-Gobilard et al., 2018)
Herkogamy Spatial separation between anthers and stigma Measurements or morphometrics Fresh or ethanol-preserved flower material Efficiency (outcross pollen transfer, Opedal, 2018)
  • Methods, type of floral material needed, and information on comparative or experimental evidence of the functional significance of certain traits in the pollination process are given. Where possible, objective (ideally quantitative) measurements on traits should be prioritised over qualitative categorisation. Information on methodological approaches is by no means exhaustive but represents examples of recent applications, more comprehensive methodology may be found in, for example, Kearns & Inouye, 1993 or the trait-specific literature.

7. There is more to pollination syndromes than a handful of floral traits

Several authors have criticised traditional pollination syndromes in being limited to a relatively narrow set of traits that are easy and fast to code (Fenster et al., 2009a; Abrahamczyk et al., 2017). This is particularly problematic in lineages in which pollination syndromes are best characterised by traits not included in traditional syndromes (Table 1; Dellinger et al., 2019a). Furthermore, quantitative traits such as colour, scent or shape are difficult to discretise and may lead to subjective, erroneous categorisation (Abrahamczyk et al., 2017). How many and which floral traits are generally included in pollination-syndrome studies? And how many of those are quantitative?

Of the 244 studies that included floral traits, the majority (77.2%) was indeed based on a relatively small set of traits (1–10; Fig. 1b). Here, 66.8% of studies included quantitative measurements of reward or attraction traits (nectar: 36.4%, scent 19.7%, colour 13.9%); 22.1% measured corolla dimensions quantitatively and 20.5% measured both sterile and reproductive floral organs quantitatively. Only 3.3% of studies were based on morphometric assessments of flower shape and only two studies quantified morphometric relationships between both corolla and reproductive organs.

These results confirmed that pollination-syndrome studies are often limited to narrow, qualitative, rather than quantitative, sets of traits. While pollinators could obviously be successfully predicted even from only a few (qualitative) traits, I argue that pollination syndromes should not be limited to pollinator prediction. By definition, pollination syndromes are multivariate in nature and (may) carry information on complex, multitrait phenotypes (Junker & Parachnowitsch, 2015). The long-lasting focus on the same narrow set of traits prevents us from exploring new areas of phenotypic evolution and gaining a more profound understanding of how flowers function. Adding new traits to studies pays off, however. For example, the relatively recent addition of quantitative assessments of floral scent through headspace techniques and gas chromatography-mass spectrometry analyses has opened up a new ‘unseen’ world (Schiestl & D?tterl, 2012). As studies in different systems are compiled, we start to see patterns of scent compounds that are potentially relevant in the communication with different functional pollinator groups and floral antagonists (Junker & Parachnowitsch, 2015). Another fruitful potential addition to pollination syndromes could be petal cell shape. Several authors have reported conical cells associated with bee pollination and flat cells found in bird-pollinated plants (Papiorek et al., 2014; Dellinger et al., 2019a). Such findings merit further investigation to evaluate whether these patterns are system specific or possibly applicable across broad taxonomic scales.

The choice of appropriate methods and metrics for the collection of phenotypic data is crucial in all biological disciplines and often limited by time constraints, equipment or available (floral) material (Table 1). Coding for traits qualitatively, as is common in pollination-syndrome research, and is fast and informative particularly when analysing large numbers of disparate species. The discretisation of biological traits always bears risks, however (Wilson et al., 2007; Abrahamczyk et al., 2017). First, discretisation may be highly subjective and circular in that categories are established for patterns (e.g. syndromes) one expects to find. Second, while character coding may be accurate when performed by the same observer, it may be difficult to repeat for a different observer. And third, trait categorisation misses the recording of within-taxon variation, which is essential to understand the adaptive potential of populations or direction/strength of selection. Thus, authors should critically evaluate whether and which traits they can record quantitatively in order to arrive with objective, comparable and repeatable datasets. Quantitative trait measurements are nowadays increasingly used for advertisement traits (Table 1; Fig. 1b) but still relatively uncommon when studying flower–pollinator fit traits. Morphometrics provides a useful approach to quantifying fit traits (Pérez et al., 2007; Kaczorowski et al., 2012; Pérez-Barrales et al., 2014; Strelin et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2017; Smith & Kriebel, 2018). To date, most authors have considered flowers in only two dimensions and focused on corolla shape only (e.g. Kaczorowski et al., 2012; Gómez et al., 2016; Smith & Kriebel, 2018; Benítez-Vieyra et al., 2019). It is high time, however, that we start studying flowers (and their pollinators) as what they are: highly three-dimensional structures. The relative position of the corolla and the reproductive organs is decisive in mediating fit with the pollinators and assuring pollen transfer. Recent advances in obtaining detailed 3D-flower models through high-resolution X-ray computed tomography (HRXCT) now provide the necessary basis for studying flower shape variation through landmark-based geometric morphometrics without losing information on the spatial arrangement of floral organs (van der Niet et al., 2010; Sedeek et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2015; Wilson et al., 2017; Dellinger et al., 2019c; Kellenberger et al., 2019; Reich et al., 2020). A wide array of methods is available and constantly being expanded, allowing for statistical analyses of multivariate quantitative trait datasets (Table 2). If, finally, authors choose to rather work with qualitative traits, they need to establish objective criteria for trait discretisation which may also be employed by future researchers working on the same system (Wilson et al., 2017; Dellinger et al., 2019a). To this end, categorisation may outperform quantification when working with morphologically or functionally complex composite traits which cannot be expressed by simple measurements (e.g. pollen-expulsion mechanism, Dellinger et al., 2019a).

Table 2. Methods for data analyses.
Purpose Questions Methods and application References
Characterising pollination syndromes Which floral traits are most powerful in separating different pollination syndromes? Which traits vary independently from pollination syndromes? Which pollination syndromes can be separated reliably with the given floral characters? Discriminant analyses: identification of floral traits which are most powerful in separating flowers pollinated by different functional pollinator groups Serrano-Serrano et al. (2017)
Random Forest analyses: examination of the contribution and behaviour of floral traits (predictor variables) in separating predefined groups (i.e. functional pollinator groups) Johnson (2013), Dellinger et al. (2019a)
Disparity of pollination syndromes or floral modules Do pollination syndromes differ in their morphological diversity, that is do certain functional pollinator groups select for more constrained (less disparate) phenotypes? Are some syndromes morphologically more constrained? Are certain parts of the flower (e.g. 'efficiency traits') more constrained than others? Calculating dissimilarity per syndrome or per floral module, testing for significant differences between syndromes or modules using PERMANOVA (nonparametric analyses of variance) Chartier et al. (2017), Dellinger et al. (2019a), Vasconcelos et al. (2019)
Visualising pollination syndromes Do species pollinated by the same functional pollinator group occupy a similar area of multivariate trait space? Do species pollinated by different functional pollinator groups occupy different areas of trait space? Principal component analysis: for traits recorded at interval scale (e.g. morphometric measurements) Strelin et al. (2016)
Non-metric multidimensional scaling, principal coordinates analysis: traits recorded at other scales (interval, ordinal, categorical), calculation of distance matrix (i.e. euclidean distances, Bray–Curtis) Wilson et al. (2007), Chartier et al. (2017), Dellinger et al. (2019a)
Summarising interaction networks Is there a relation between pollination syndrome and connectivity or nestedness? Which floral traits contribute to specialization and/or network modularity in different plant communities? How does (human-induced) environmental change affect interaction networks? Network analyses (calculate e.g. modularity, specialization, connectivity, nestedness), bi-partite networks of flower and pollinator traits, Machine Learning algorithms Olesen et al. (2007), Watts et al. (2016), Pichler et al. (2020)
Predicting pollinators What is the most likely pollinator of a certain plant species? Discriminant analyses: predicting pollinators through the most discriminant floral traits of species with known pollinators (see above) Martén-Rodríguez et al. (2009), Lagomarsino et al. (2017), Serrano-Serrano et al. (2017)
Cluster analyses, Machine Learning (e.g. Random Forest analyses: classification through Machine Learning, Random Forest is trained on floral trait data for species with known pollinators, then pollinators are predicted for species without pollinator information) Johnson (2013), Abrahamszyk et al. (2017), Dellinger et al. (2019a), Pichler et al. (2020)
Expert coding by a single trait (or multiple traditional syndrome traits) Lagomarsino et al. (2017), Fragoso-Martínez et al. (2018)
Testing/validating pollination syndromes Do floral traits and pollinator assemblages correlate? Correlation analyses (e.g. Mantel test) on distance matrices or bi-partite networks of flower and pollinator traits and Machine Learning algorithms Smith et al. (2008), Martén-Rodtríguez et al. (2009), Johnson (2013), Pichler et al. (2020)
Does prediction by different floral trait sets yield the same result? Validate classification through the construction of a morphospace based on traits not used for classification (i.e. if syndromes classified on colour, exclude colour from validation) Lagomarsino et al. (2017)
Are pollination syndromes reliable tools to predicting pollinators? Validate syndrome prediction through empirical observations in the field Armbruster et al. (2011)
Evolution of pollination syndromes What is the ancestral pollination syndrome? How often have shifts in pollination syndrome occurred? Is there a recurring shift directionality? Are there any reversals? Are some syndromes more likely to shift than others? Ancestral state reconstruction of selected floral traits or pollination syndromes on phylogeny; when reconstructing deep nodes, heritable floral traits may be more reliable than evolutionarily labile pollination syndromes (van der Niet et al., 2014) Lagomarsino et al. (2017), Serrano-Serrano et al. (2017), Dellinger et al. (2019a)
Do floral traits respond to pollinator-mediated selection? Are pollination syndromes associated with different evolutionary optima? Do flowers pollinated by the same functional pollinator group converge in the same optimum? Do flowers with different secondary pollinators evolve towards different optima? Phylogenetic comparative methods (e.g. Brownian motion, Ornstein–Uhlenbeck models) Smith et al. (2008), Strelin et al. (2016), Lagomarsino et al. (2017), Smith & Kriebel (2018)
What is the impact of pollinator shifts on plant diversification? Do certain functional pollinator groups increase speciation or extinction rates? Are certain (combinations of) pollinators evolutionarily more successful? Do different floral trait clusters evolve at different rates? What is the relative importance of other biotic interactions (e.g. competition for pollinators) and abiotic conditions in clade diversification? Modelling speciation/extinction/diversification in relation to pollination syndromes Muchhala et al. (2014), Lagomarsino et al. (2016), Serrano-Serrano et al. (2017), Kriebel et al. (2019), Wessinger et al. (2019)
How do flowers adapt to different pollinator selection regimes? Are certain floral parts/modules more evolvable and evolve at different rates? Can pollinator shifts disrupt within-flower correlation/covariance structures? Are some syndromes more evolvable than others? Geometric morphometrics of flower shape and multivariate phylogenetic comparative analyses Dellinger et al. (2019c), Reich et al. (2020)
  • Overview of questions which can be addressed using pollination syndromes and a nonexhaustive compilation of methods for trait and syndrome data analyses and recent applications. Note that when testing the evolution of syndromes and traits, robust phylogenies are a prerequisite; difficulties of inferring phylogenies (i.e. due to incomplete lineage sorting or ancient hybridisation) are not dealt with here.

8. Not all floral traits are equally informative

Not all floral traits are affected equally by (pollinator-mediated) selection (Pérez et al., 2007; Pérez-Barrales et al., 2014; Fenster et al., 2015, Caruso et al., 2018), and not all traits are equally important in each pollination syndrome (Johnson, 2013; Zung et al., 2015; Dellinger et al., 2019a). Despite this knowledge, traditional pollination syndromes often lump together all traits without further analysing differences in specificity and function. Are there (traditional syndrome) traits that are commonly recorded but bear little predictive power?

In total, 77 of the 97 studies estimating pollinator efficiency recorded floral traits. The traits most commonly assessed were (mostly nectar) reward (62.3%), timing or duration of anthesis/sexual phase (48.1%), scent (35.0%), corolla length (32.5%), corolla width (29.9%) and colour (29.8%; Fig. 5a; Table S3). Interestingly, 56 of these studies reported some traits as being particularly informative in characterising the specific pollination syndrome or in discriminating visitors from pollinators (Fig. 5b; Table S4). Nectar (39.3%), colour (30.4%), scent (26.8%), flower size (19.6%) and anthesis patterns (16.1%) were reported as important traits most often. In addition, 25 studies stated which traits were uninformative or misleading in syndrome classification (Fig. 5c; Table S5), with colour (52.0%) being most uninformative/confounding (i.e. contradicting traditional syndrome expectations), followed by scent (32.0%), shape (24.0%) and reward (16.0%).

Details are in the caption following the image
Floral traits studied and reported as informative or uninformative/misleading in differentiating/characterising syndromes; advertisement traits appear in red colours, efficiency traits in blue colours. (a) Reward, anthesis, scent, colour, corolla length, corolla tube width and pollen : ovule ratios were studied most often. (b) Reward (nectar), colour, scent and corolla tube width were reported as most relevant in characterising syndromes while (c) colour, scent and flower shape were reported as misleading/indicating other pollinators than observed. (d) Proportional comparison of (a–c).

Clearly, these results have to be treated with care, as traits have not been recorded/analysed the same way. However, it is noteworthy that nectar rewards are assessed most often and reported as highly informative, while colour is assessed only half as often but reported as most uninformative/misleading (Fig. 5d). Flower colour undoubtedly is highly important in many systems (Dudash et al., 2011; Johnson, 2013; Rodrigues et al., 2018), but not in all. Selection studies have shown, for example, that flower colour is not selected by pollinators, while display size and plant height are (Silene, Brothers & Atwell, 2014; Fenster et al., 2015). Indeed, as an advertisement trait, colour may also underlie selection by antagonists (avoidance of herbivores, Camargo et al., 2018) or pollinator competition (Muchhala et al., 2014). It would be too early (and most likely wrong) to dismiss any trait from pollination syndromes. Instead, I argue that we should focus on traits which do and do not match syndrome expectations alike. Trait mismatch may come from complex biotic interactions, but also be a mere consequence of a species’ phylogenetic background (developmental/genetic constraints) and the limited number of trait combinations that could evolve (O’Meara et al., 2016). By systematically evaluating the relative importance of a large number of traits and quantifying (mis)match with traditional syndromes, we may better pinpoint the traits important in adaptation to pollinators in each study system and also understand how adaptation can be achieved through new trait combinations (Dellinger et al., 2014, Pichler et al., 2020).

9. Analysing floral evolution and diversity in the context of pollination syndromes

To date, our understanding of how flowers evolve and adapt to pollinators remains fragmentary. From a functional perspective, flowers are highly synorganised (integrated) structures in which different organs co-function to achieve flower protection, pollinator attraction and rewarding as well as pollen transfer, fertilisation and seed development (Armbruster, 2014; Diggle, 2014). Despite this overall (functional) integration, the above-mentioned processes may influence different parts of the flower differently, potentially leading to different (functional) modules within the flower (Armbruster, 2014; Esteve-Altava, 2017). Recent macroevolutionary analyses have indeed demonstrated that such functional modules may evolve independently of each other, that is along different trajectories and at different rates, in response to pollinator-mediated selection (Opedal, 2019; Dellinger et al., 2019c; Reich et al., 2020). This relative evolutionary independence of functional modules has been proposed to facilitate (and hence explain the frequent occurrence of) shifts between pollination syndromes in closely related plant species (Armbruster, 2014). Several authors have suggested that a single mutation of large effect in advertisement traits (i.e. colour or scent) suffices to induce a pollinator shift, as it may lead to the attraction of a different pollinator (Bradshaw & Schemske, 2003; Wessinger et al., 2014; Wessinger & Rausher, 2015; Sheehan et al., 2016 there were only two studies that calculated disparity). Alternatively, scenarios have been proposed in which strong (extrinsic) alterations in the pollinator community (i.e. environmental perturbation, colonisation of new habitat) render a new pollinator more efficient and flowers gradually adapt to this new pollinator, that is through optimisation of efficiency traits (Thomson & Wilson, 2008; Kay et al., 2019). What is the likelihood of either of these scenarios? Do advertisement traits consistently evolve faster than and change before efficiency traits? How important are changes in advertisement traits in mediating reproductive isolation in sympatry compared to allopatry? And to what extent are such trait changes independent (modular) or integrated?

Molecular developmental studies, experimental manipulations, selection studies and QTL screening have the capacity to get to the core of these questions (Bolstad et al., 2014; Wessinger et al., 2014). In recent years, particular focus has been placed on studying the advertisement traits colour and scent (each trait included in 33% of studies in the literature review, but also see Kostyun et al., 2019). In several pollination-syndrome model systems (e.g. Antirrhinum, Mimulus, Penstemon, Petunia), these traits are controlled by few genes and trait modification have had large effects on pollinator attraction. Through the creation of floral phenotypes intermediate between bee and moth pollination, for example, Dell’Olivo & Kuhlemeier (2013) could show that colour change significantly increased pollination by the new pollinator, supporting the idea that single mutations of large effect could trigger pollinator shifts. Furthermore, changes in colour have also brought about a change in other functional traits such as nectar guides (affecting the correct positioning of pollinators) or petal cell shapes (reviewed by Yuan et al., 2014). Such observed correlations have been attributed to pleiotropy (e.g. reward and corolla shape in Penstemon, Wessinger et al., 2014; flower size and nectar in Jaltomata, Kostyun et al., 2019) or to linked, but independent, gene clusters (e.g. colour, scent and reproductive organ length in Petunia, Hermann et al., 2013; Kostyun et al., 2019). Both processes allow the rapid adaptation of multiple traits to changing pollinator selection regimes.

The above-mentioned studies linking phenotypic to genotypic variation are restricted to a limited number of model systems that can be propagated and crossed in great numbers (Smith et al., 2020). The vast majority of biological systems and plant–pollinator interactions is not part of this group, however. Phylogenetic comparative methods offer a powerful tool to analysing tempo, mode and mechanisms of floral trait evolution in these systems (Smith & Kriebel, 2018; Wessinger et al., 2019; Dellinger et al., 2019c). In recent years, authors have often tested the importance of a single or a few isolated traits in explaining diversification (Lagomarsino et al., 2016). For understanding mechanisms and patterns of flower evolution and diversification in relation to pollination, however, multivariate trait datasets are needed. (Geometric) morphometrics are particularly promising for testing different hypotheses of shape evolution in flowers. While such tests have a long tradition in anthropology and zoology (Esteve-Altava, 2017), this is not the case in plants. This is somewhat surprising as monophyletic plant lineages with repeated independent shifts into different pollination syndromes offer the optimal set-up to testing the role of developmental constraints and functional adaptation under divergent (pollinator) selection regimes. How do pollinator shifts affect correlation structures within the flower? Is there developmental, functional or evolutionary linkage between floral organs/modules? Are certain intrafloral modules more integrated than others and (how) do they differ in their rate of evolution? Are these patterns different between different pollination syndromes? Although testing of these questions at macroevolutionary scales in plants is in its infancy, first patterns start arising. First, efficiency traits involved in the direct physical interaction with pollinators show particularly strong signals of selection and correlated (integrated) evolution (Opedal, 2018, 2019; Benítez-Vieyra et al., 2019; Dellinger et al., 2019c; Reich et al., 2020; compared with Caruso et al., 2018, also see Fig. 5d). Second, pollinator shifts could disrupt both type and strength of correlation structures within flowers and change functional modules (Dellinger et al., 2019c; Reich et al., 2020). And third, the modularity of specialised flowers was found to be structured by function, while modularity of generalised flowers was structured by developmental affinities of floral organs (Reich et al., 2020). It will be interesting to relate these results to advertisement traits to understand more deeply their interplay during pollinator shifts and, potentially, speciation.

Another concept that ties in here is the concept of evolvability. The standing genetic variation of a trait under selection determines its potential to adapt, and hence to constrain or facilitate shifts in pollination syndrome (Opedal, 2019). Rarely have pollination biologists aimed at quantifying this variation and associated evolutionary potential (Armbruster, 2014). What is the relationship between floral phenotypic variation/diversity (flower disparity, Chartier et al., 2017), evolvability and functional pollinator groups? Are flowers adapted to certain functional pollinator groups more disparate than flowers adapted to other groups? Are ancestral pollination syndromes (estimated from extant taxa) more disparate than shifted syndromes? And is disparity (as a measure of evolvability) in the ancestral pollination syndrome a prerequisite for pollinator shifts (Opedal, 2019)? Although disparity could be calculated and analysed easily from multivariate trait datasets (31.1% of studies in the literature reviewed contained multivariate analyses), there were only two studies that calculated disparity (Dellinger et al., 2019a; Vasconcelos et al., 2019). These studies found high disparity in two large bee-pollinated clades (Melastomataceae, Myrtaceae), but this led to pollinator shifts in only one clade (Dellinger et al., 2019a). Disparity of the shifted syndromes was significantly lower (Dellinger et al., 2019a).

Earlier this year, the extension of comparative methods to genotype-to-phenotype mapping approaches through the development of new software tools has been proposed (Smith et al., 2020). This extension holds tremendous and exciting potential for pollination syndromes to more deeply investigate the questions and patterns outlined above in nonmodel organisms (Smith et al., 2020).

III. Merianieae as a case study – pollination syndromes and flower evolution

In our study on floral evolution in Merianieae (Dellinger et al., 2019a), we aimed to describe pollination syndromes at the tribal level (Fig. 6). Hence, we expanded trait coding beyond traditional syndrome traits and included 61 floral traits that were potentially relevant for pollination in the group. By contrast with many other studies (see section II.6 ‘Prediction of pollinators’), syndrome predictions were inaccurate when using only a subset (colour, size, shape) of traditional syndrome traits (Fig. 6e; Tables S6, S7). When including the full set of characteristics used in traditional syndromes, the same three syndromes (‘buzz bee’, ‘passerine’, ‘mixed-vertebrate’) could be distinguished, as with the 61 traits (Fig. 5e). Analysis of the 3D flower shape space through geometric morphometrics in Merianieae yielded the same three syndromes (Dellinger et al., 2019c).

Details are in the caption following the image
Exemplary workflow for establishing and studying pollination syndromes at low taxonomic scales (tribe Merianieae, Melastomataceae). (a) Documenting pollinator (and visitor) assemblages throughout anthesis, ideally quantitatively (Supporting Information Notes S1), should be the first step when establishing a new study system. (b) Recording a large set of system-specific, potentially pollination-relevant floral traits (Table 1). Advantages of objective quantitative trait coding have been demonstrated (Abrahamczyk et al., 2017). (c) Characterising and describing pollination syndromes for species with known pollinators using objective methods (i.e. Machine Learning approaches, Pichler et al., 2020). These methods may also be used to validate syndrome delimitation by testing the predictive power on species with known pollinators (Tables 2, S7). (d) Based on syndromes characterised in step (c), predictions of pollinators may be made for species where no pollinator observations are available or feasible, that is to study pollination syndromes across speciose clades (Table 2). (e) Further analyses of pollination syndromes may include calculation of disparity (morphological diversity), testing different hypotheses of floral modularity or studying floral evolution and angiosperm diversification (Table 2). Here, three different morphospaces have been calculated to exemplify how differences in the number and type of traits studied affect syndrome delimitation. While flower colour, size and corolla shape are not sufficient in differentiating syndromes, grouping into syndromes became clear when using 14 traditional pollination syndrome characters and 61 Merianieae specific characters (Tables S6, S8). Pollination syndromes may further be used to analyse flower evolution more deeply: here, a hypothesis of floral modularity, partitioning the flower into three functional modules, is presented as well as the results of modelling 3D-flower-shape evolution under an Ornstein–Uhlenbeck process to test for convergence under distinct pollinator selection regimes. All presented data, syndrome classification, morphospace calculation and traitgram are derived from Dellinger et al. (2019a,c). Pictures of flowers stem from Dellinger et al. (2019a), pictures of pollinators have been taken by Dellinger with the exception of the bat pollination picture ? Nathan Muchhala, used with permission. Coloration in morphospaces is based on syndrome predictions from Dellinger et al. (2019a).

Through Machine Learning approaches, we found that several traits that were not included in traditional syndromes but specific to Merianieae, such as the pollen-expulsion mechanism or structural stamen properties, were most important in differentiating syndromes (Table 1; Fig. 6e). Specifically, buzz pollination with associated tubular anthers is ancestral in Merianieae and pollen can only be released through vibrations produced by bees (Dellinger et al., 2019a). Although vertebrates cannot vibrate flowers, the tubular anther structure typical for buzz syndrome was retained during shifts to vertebrate pollination (Dellinger et al., 2019a). This mismatch between trait and syndrome expectation is highly interesting as it possibly reflects a developmental constraint and led to the evolution of complex alternative pollen-expulsion mechanisms (Dellinger et al., 2014).

The availability of a large trait dataset and a (dated) phylogeny made it possible to analyse flower evolution in Merianieae more deeply. First, we tested one basic assumption of the pollination-syndrome concept that has been tested surprisingly infrequently: that flowers pollinated by different functional groups underlie divergent selection regimes. Indeed, OU models, explaining adaptation under different selection regimes, fitted the data best (Fig. 6e; Dellinger et al., 2019c). Second, we tested different hypotheses on developmental or functional modularity of Merianieae flowers (Fig. 6e). These analyses revealed that all flowers are structured by functional adaptations to their specific pollinators and that different organ types can make up a functional module. The ‘buzz bee’ syndrome was the most modular (particularly in corolla shape). Modularity was significantly weaker in the two vertebrate syndromes in which corolla shape has acquired a new pollinator fit function (also see Table S8). Finally, analysis of the rate of evolution between advertisement and efficiency module showed that advertisement traits evolve around. six times faster than efficiency traits (Fig. 6e; Dellinger et al., 2019c).

IV. Concluding remarks

Much discussion in the last decades has lingered on the same problems regarding the validity and predictive potential of pollination syndromes. My review demonstrates that some of these problems (i.e. predictive inaccuracy, visitation by multiple functional groups) have been solved by refining techniques, while others (i.e. few traits studied) prevail. Overall, I argue that pollination syndromes are a robust concept to accurately circumscribe plant–pollinator relationships both at large and small scales, as long as the longstanding caveats are acknowledged. I emphasise that each study system has its specific discriminant traits, which the researcher has to identify before employing any categorisation or syndrome prediction. Hence, instead of focusing our discussion on whether a flower corresponds to a specific traditional syndrome, we may rather want to quantify the reason, extent and way by which it deviates from a traditional syndrome. Sophisticated statistical tools help us to approach these questions and provide remedy against typological classification (Table 2). In any case, an essential prerequisite is the detailed and objective (ideally quantitative) assessment of flower and pollinator data.

Furthermore, I emphasise that pollination syndromes, as a multivariate concept, should not be limited to pollinator prediction. Particularly the combination of broad trait datasets with quantitative pollinator data and molecular/phylogenetic comparative methods holds tremendous potential for more deeply understanding mechanisms of floral evolution through selection, developmental/genetic constraints and trade-offs. Studies on flower trait functioning have shown that traits involved in pollinator attraction are evolutionarily labile and underlie weaker selection compared with traits mediating physical interaction with pollinators. In this respect, pollination syndromes will be important to extrapolate findings, that is whether certain functional trait clusters are particularly strongly selected by certain functional groups (or generalist assemblages) but not by others, and whether these findings are consistent in diverse plant lineages and communities in different parts of the world. Finally, pollination syndromes are also an important tool in conservation and may serve, for example, for large-scale analyses of the effect of environmental perturbations on functional diversity.

Acknowledgements

I thank Jürg Sch?nenberger and Stacey Smith for the encouragement to write this review, Jürg Sch?nenberger and three anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier versions of the manuscript, Susana Magallón for proposing this review to the New Phytologist board of editors, and Marion Chartier for stimulating discussions on pollination syndromes and data analyses. This work was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) grant P 30669-B29.

      输卵管堵塞吃什么药可以疏通 梦见换房子是什么预兆 时柱比肩是什么意思 异国他乡的意思是什么 宝宝舌苔白厚是什么原因
      桑葚有什么功效和作用 结肠炎有什么症状表现 黄鼻涕是什么原因 舌苔红是什么原因 乙肝15阳性什么意思
      赛治是什么药 台风什么时候到福建 来月经有异味什么原因 crs是什么意思 什么情况需要打破伤风
      胰岛素偏高是什么意思 1999属什么 白菜什么时候种 脚后跟痛什么原因 成人晚上磨牙是什么原因
      空囊是什么意思hcv8jop1ns4r.cn 榴莲吃了对身体有什么好处hcv9jop4ns4r.cn 德国纳粹是什么意思hcv7jop9ns3r.cn 雌性激素是什么hcv9jop6ns6r.cn 什么是pinhcv9jop8ns1r.cn
      高密度脂蛋白胆固醇偏低是什么意思hcv7jop9ns1r.cn 地图舌吃什么药hcv9jop4ns3r.cn asks是什么意思hcv9jop5ns5r.cn 送同学什么毕业礼物好hcv8jop6ns0r.cn 梦是什么意思hcv8jop3ns1r.cn
      肝穿刺检查是什么意思hcv9jop8ns3r.cn 人乳头瘤病毒是什么病hcv9jop4ns4r.cn 乳酸菌素片什么时候吃hcv9jop0ns0r.cn 感冒口苦是什么原因hcv9jop1ns4r.cn 北京市长是什么级别hcv9jop5ns4r.cn
      吃什么对卵巢有好处hcv9jop8ns2r.cn 施食是什么意思hcv8jop4ns2r.cn 原发性高血压什么意思hcv9jop3ns8r.cn 11月7号是什么星座hcv8jop6ns5r.cn 茱萸是什么东西hcv8jop2ns9r.cn
      百度